In addition, in many languages, sex is often closely correlated to the basic modified form (lemma) of the name, and sometimes a name can be changed to produce (for example) male and female words of a similar meaning. See below: Morphological criteria based on form. Although the second sentence may appear poorly grammatically incorrect (constructio ad sensum), it is common in the language. With one or more sentences in between, the second form becomes even more likely. However, it is never possible to switch to natural sex with articles and pronouns or adjectives. Thus, it is never fair to say a girl („a girl“ with an indeterminate woman article) or a little girl with a female demonstrator and an adjective. Note that, in our previous studies, articles (written or spoken) that followed gender line drawings, generated negativity similar to N400, contrary to the widespread positivity between 500 and 700 msec observed in this study. This difference cannot be explained by the article itself, because the sentences written in Wicha, Moreno et al. (2003) were identical to those used here, except that the targets here were words; in the previous one, they were line drawings. It is remarkable, though perhaps fortuitous, that in each study, the polarity of the effects of gender agreement on nostantin and gender expectations were the same in the article – both were negative for sentences with embedded and positive line drawings for words (see Figure 7). Violations of the agreement between men and women on the drawings gave a negative, albeit weak, result in the field of the P600; the distribution of the scalp was more frontal and biased towards the left hemisphere than the current results. Therefore, while the timing of treatment of agreements between men and women seems similar for words and images, the nature of the processes involved seems more different. Although the current study was not designed to correct the differences in processing between words and images, we will show some possible reasons for this difference in polarity in the effects of concordance, which must be due in one way or another to the target modality – image for word – and/or the associated treatment needs (e.g.B.
The treatment of rebus against normal sentences). Thus, the current simple form of the verb RISE must increase in the plural with plural theme prices, but with the singular the price must be singular increases. In this case, the shape of the subject varies according to the two meanings (something grammars call „Concord“), but in other cases, only one of the two words will change shape. Keywords: grammatical gender, chord, lexico-syntaxic characteristic, language production, ERP Grammatical sex of a no bite voice does not always correspond to its natural sex. An example is the German word for girl; this derives from the maid „maiden“ as a „girl“ with the small suffix, and this suffix always makes the no bite with grammatically castration. Therefore, the grammatical sex of girls is neutered, although its natural sex is female (because it refers to a woman).